Nowa wersja platformy, zawierająca wyłącznie zasoby pełnotekstowe, jest już dostępna.
Przejdź na https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników
2006 | 2 | 2-3 | 113-130
Tytuł artykułu

Practicing perfection: How concert soloists prepare for performance

Wybrane pełne teksty z tego czasopisma
Warianty tytułu
Języki publikacji
Abstrakty
EN
Musical performances by concert soloists in the Western classical tradition are normally memorized. For memory to work reliably under the pressures of the concert stage, the performance must be practiced until it is thoroughly automatic. At the same time, the performance must be fresh and spontaneous in order to communicate emotionally with the audience. The resolution of this apparent contradiction is provided by longitudinal case studies of concert soloists preparing new works for performance. Like expert memorists in other domains, experienced musicians use highly practiced retrieval schemes to accomplish their extraordinary feats of memory. Performers have a mental map of the piece in mind as they perform that tells them where they are and what comes next - a series of landmarks, hierarchically organized by the sections and subsections of the music. The musician attends to theseperformance cuesin order to ensure that the performance unfolds as planned. Performance cues are established by thinking about a particular feature of the music during practice so that it later comes to mind automatically. Performance cues help the soloist consciously monitor and control the rapid, automatic actions of playing, while adjusting to the needs of the moment. During practice, the musician attends mostly tobasic performance cuesrepresenting critical technical features (e.g., fingerings), andinterpretive performance cues, representing phrasings, and changes in dynamics, tempo, and timbre. During performance, the musician hopes to attend mainly toexpressive performance cuesrepresenting the musical feelings to be conveyed to the audience (e.g., excitement). We illustrate this analysis with a typical case study of a concert pianist learning J.S. Bach'sItalian Concerto (Presto).
Słowa kluczowe
Rocznik
Tom
2
Numer
2-3
Strony
113-130
Opis fizyczny
Twórcy
  • University of Connecticut
autor
  • University of Connecticut
Bibliografia
  • Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. J., & Broadbent, M. H. (1978). A comparison of hierarchical matrix retrieval schemes in recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 486-497.
  • Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (1997). "Pulling teeth and torture": Musical memory and problem solving.Thinking and Reasoning: Special Issue on Expert Thinking, 3, 315-336.
  • Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., & Crawford, M. (2002).Practicing perfection: Memory and piano performance.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (2001). A comparison of practice and self-report as sources of information about the goals of expert practice.Psychology of Music, 29, 39-69.
  • Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (2002). Practicing perfection: Piano performance as expert memory.Psychological Science, 13, 342-349.
  • Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., Lemieux, A., & Chen, C. (2003). "Seeing the big picture": Piano practice as expert problem solving.Music Perception, 20, 461-485.
  • Chaffin, R., Lemieux, A., & Chen, C. (2004) "It's different each time I play": Why highly polished performances vary. In S. D. Lipscomb, R. Ashley, R. O. Gjerdingen & P. Webster (Eds.),Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition.Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions.
  • Chaffin, R., Lemieux, A., & Chen, C.(2006). Spontaneity and creativity in highly practiced performance. In I. Deliège & G. A. Wiggins,Musical creativity: Multidisciplinary research in theory and practice(pp. 200-218). London: Psychology Press.
  • Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skilled and working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation16 (pp. 1-58). New York: Academic Press.
  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind's eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.),Visual information processing(pp. 215-281). New York: Academic Press.
  • Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Cognitive Science, 5, 121-125.
  • Clarke, E. (1999). Rhythm and timing in music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.),The Psychology of music(pp. 473-500). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Clarke, E. (1989). The perception of expressive timing in music.Psychological Research, 51, 2-9.
  • Cooke, J. F. (1999).Great pianists on piano playing: Godowsky, Hofmann, Lhévinne, Paderewski, and 24 other legendary performers.Toronto: Dover (originally published 1913, expanded edition published 1917).
  • DePoli, G., Roda, A. & Vidolin, A. (1998). Note-by-note analysis of the influence of expressive intentions and musical structure in violin performance.Journal of New Music Research, 27, 293-321.
  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory.Psychological Review, 10, 211-245.
  • Ericsson, K. A., & Oliver, W. L. (1989). A methodology for assessing the detailed structure of memory skills. In A. M. Colley & J. R. Beech (Eds.),Acquisition and performance of cognitive skills(pp. 193-215). Chichester: Wiley.
  • Gabrielsson, A. (1999). The performance of music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.),The psychology of music(2nd ed.) (pp. 579-602). San Diego: Academic Press.
  • Gabrielsson, A., & Lindström, E. (2001). The influence of musical structure on emotion. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.),Music and emotion: Theory and research.New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Ginsborg, J., Chaffin, R., & Nicholson, G. (2006). Shared performance cues in singing and conducting: A content analysis of talk during practice.Psychology of Music, 34, 167-194.
  • Gilden, D. L. (2001). Cognitive emissions of 1/f noise.Psychological Review, 108, 33-56.
  • Glaser, R., & Chi, M. (1988). Overview. In M. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. Farr (Eds.),The nature of expertise(pp. XV-XXVIII). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look-ahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from grand-master-level chess.Psychological Science, 7, 52-55.
  • Hallam, S. (1995a). Professional musicians' approaches to the learning and interpretation of music.Psychology of Music, 23, 111-128.
  • Hallam, S. (1995b). Professional musicians' orientation to practice: Implications for teaching.British Journal of Music Education, 12, 3-19.
  • Halpern, A. R., & Bower, G. H. (1982). Musical expertise and melodic structure in memory for musical notation.American Journal of Psychology, 95, 31-50.
  • Hinson, M. (1987).Guide to the Pianist's Repertoire.Bloomington: Indiana University Press
  • Imreh, G. pianist. (1996).J. S. Bach[CD]. New York: Connoisseur Society.
  • Juslin, P. N. (2000). Cue utilization in communication of emotion in music performance: Relating performance to perception.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1797-1813.
  • Juslin, P. N. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code?Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770-814.
  • Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 25, 331-359.
  • Latash, M. L., Scholz, J. P., & Schoner, G. (2002). Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability.Exercise and Sports Science Reviews, 30, 26-31.
  • Lisboa, T., Chaffin, R., Schiaroli, A. G., & Barrera, A. (2004). Investigating practice and performance on the cello. In S. D. Lipscomb, R. Ashley, R. O. Gjerdingen & P. Webster (Eds.),Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition, Evanston, Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions.
  • Mach, E. (1991).Great contemporary pianists speak for themselves.New York: Dover.
  • Mandler, G., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Free and constrained concept learning and subsequent recall.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 126-131.
  • Miklaszewski, K. (1989). A case study of a pianist preparing a musical performance.Psychology of Music, 17, 95-109.
  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
  • Neuhaus, H. (1973).The art of piano playing.New York: Praeger Publishers Inc.
  • Noice, H., Chaffin, R., Jeffrey, J., & Noice, T. (in press). Memorization by a jazz pianist: A case study.Music Psychology.
  • Noyle, L. J. (Ed.). (1987).Pianists on piano playing: Interviews with twelve concert pianists.Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow.
  • Palmer, C., & Pfordresher, P. (2003). Incremental planning in sequence production.Psychological Review, 110, 683-712.
  • Palmer, C. (1997). Music performance.Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 115-138.
  • Palmer, C. (1989). Mapping musical thought to musical performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 331-346.
  • Raaijmakers, J. G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory.Psychological Review, 88, 93-134.
  • Repp, B. H. (1995). Quantitative effects of global tempo on expressive timing in music performance: Some perceptual evidence.Music Perception, 13, 39-57.
  • Repp, B. H. (1998). Obligatory "expectations" of expressive timing induced by perception of musical structure.Psychological Research, 61, 33-43.
  • Roediger, H. L., III, & Crowder, R. C. (1976). A serial position effect in recall of United States presidents.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8, 275-278.
  • Shaffer, L. H. (1984). Timing in solo and duet piano performances.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,36A, 577-595.
  • Shaffer, L. H., Clarke, E. F., & Todd, N. P. (1985). Meter and rhythm in piano playing.Cognition, 20, 61-77.
  • Stins, F. J., & Michaels, C. F. (1999). Strategy differences in oscillatory tracking: Stimulus-hand versus stimulus manipulandum coupling.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1793-1812.
  • Sundberg, J., Friberg, A., & Frydén, L. (1991). Threshold and preference quantities of rules for music performance.Music Perception, 9, 71-92.
  • Thompson, C. P., Cowan, T. M., & Frieman, J. (1993).Memory Search by a Memorist.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Todorov, E., & Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination.Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1226-1235.
  • Tulving, E. (1962). Subjective organization in free recall of "unrelated" words.Psychological Review, 69, 344-354.
  • Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 331-350.
  • Williamon, A., & Valentine, E. (2002). The role of retrieval structures in memorizing music.Cognitive Psychology, 44, 1-32.
  • Williamon, A., Valentine, E., & Valentine, J. (2002). Shifting the focus of attention between levels of musical structure.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 493-520.
Typ dokumentu
Bibliografia
Identyfikatory
Identyfikator YADDA
bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0050-z
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.