Czasopismo
Tytuł artykułu
Wybrane pełne teksty z tego czasopisma
Warianty tytułu
Języki publikacji
Abstrakty
When objects denoted by target words are classified as belonging to a certain category (e.g., to be either small or large) responding is faster when the target word is preceded by a masked prime word belonging to the same rather than a different category. Recently, there has been some controversy on whether such masked priming effects are confined to primes that are practised as targets as well, or whether they transfer to other novel prime words. We report data which show that the transfer of unconscious priming to unpractised stimuli depends on the size of the target set. Priming does transfer to novel (unpractised) primes with a large target set (40 different target words), whereas no transfer to novel primes occurs with a small target set (4 different target words). We conclude that the size and structure of the target set crucially determine the way participants handle a task and thus, determine how unconscious stimuli are processed.
Słowa kluczowe
Czasopismo
Rocznik
Tom
Numer
Strony
37-45
Opis fizyczny
Twórcy
autor
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians University of Würzburg, Germany
autor
- Department of Psychology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Germany
autor
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians University of Würzburg, Germany
autor
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians University of Würzburg, Germany
Bibliografia
- Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000). Parts outweigh the whole (word) in unconscious analysis of meaning.Psychological Science, 11, 118-124.
- Ach, N. (1905).Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.
- Ansorge, U., Heumann, M., & Scharlau, I. (2002). Influences of visibility, intentions, and probability in a peripheral cuing task.Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 528-545.
- Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Pizzamiglio, L., D'Amico, S., & Hernandez, A. (1995). Gender and lexical access in Italian.Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 847-862.
- Damian, M. F. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 154-165.
- Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec'H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P.-F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming.Nature, 395, 597-600.
- Dell' Acqua, R., & Grainger, J. (1999). Unconscious semantic priming from pictures.Cognition, 73, B1-B15.
- Galton, F. (1880). Visualised numerals.Nature, 21, 252-256.
- Göbel, S., Walsh, V., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2001). The mental number line and the human angular gyrus.NeuroImage, 14, 1278-1289.
- Greenwald, A. G., Abrams, R. L., Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). Long-term semantic memory versus contextual memory in unconscious number processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 235-247.
- Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S.C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation.Science, 273, 1699-1702.
- Klinger, M. R., Burton, P. C., & Pitts, G. S. (2000). Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. response competition, not spreading activation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 441-455.
- Koechlin, E., Naccache, L., Block, E., & Dehaene, S. (1999). Primed numbers: Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1882-1905.
- Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition.Cognition, 88, 223-242.
- Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (in press). On the masking and disclosure of unconscious elaborate processing. A reply to Van Opstal, Reynvoet, & Verguts (2005).Cognition.
- Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization.Psychological Review, 95, 492-527.
- Mattler, U. (2003). Priming of mental operations by masked stimuli.Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 167-187.
- Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli.Cognition, 80, 223-237.
- Naccache, L., Gaillard, R., Adam, C., Hasboun, D., Clemenceau, S., Baulac, M., Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2005). A direct intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words.PNAS, 102, 7713-7717.
- Neumann, O. (1989). On the origins and status of the concept of automatic processing.Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 197, 411-428.
- Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.),Attention and performance XV. Conscious and nonconscious information processing(pp. 123-150). Cambridge: MIT Press.
- Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Automatic stimulus-response associations may be semantically mediated.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 107-112.
- Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002).E-prime user's guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
- Snodgrass, M., Bernat, E., & Shevrin, H. (2004). Unconscious perception: A model based approach to method and evidence.Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 846-867.
- Van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B., & Verguts, T. (in press). How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003).Cognition.
Typ dokumentu
Bibliografia
Identyfikatory
Identyfikator YADDA
bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0043-y